Our brains give us the remarkable ability to make sense of situations we鈥檝e never encountered before鈥攁 familiar person in an unfamiliar place, for example, or a coworker in a different job role鈥攂ut the mechanism our brains use to accomplish this has been a longstanding mystery of neuroscience.
Now, researchers at the University of Colorado Boulder have demonstrated that our brains could process these new situations by relying on a method similar to the 鈥減ointer鈥 system used by computers. 鈥淧ointers鈥 are used to tell a computer where to look for information stored elsewhere in the system to replace a variable.
For the study, published today in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, the research team relied on sentences with words used in unique ways to test the brain鈥檚 ability to understand the role familiar words play in a sentence even when those words are used in unfamiliar, and even nonsensical, ways.听
For example, in the sentence, 鈥淚 want to desk you,鈥 we understand the word 鈥渄esk鈥 is being used as a verb even though our past experience with the word 鈥渄esk鈥 is as a noun.
鈥淭he fact that you understand that the sentence is grammatically well formed means you can process these completely novel inputs,鈥 said Randall O鈥橰eilly, a professor in CU-Boulder鈥檚 Department of Psychology and Neuroscience and co-author of the study. 鈥淏ut in the past when we鈥檝e tried to get computer models of a brain to do that, we haven鈥檛 been successful.鈥
This shows that human brains are able to understand the sentence as a structure with variables鈥攁 subject, a verb and often, an object鈥攁nd that the brain can assign a wide variety of words to those variables and still understand the sentence structure. But the way the brain does this has not been understood.
Computers routinely complete similar tasks. In computer science, for example, a computer program could create an email form letter that has a pointer in the greeting line. The pointer would then draw the name information for each individual recipient into the greeting being sent to that person.
In the new study, led by Trenton Kriete, a postdoctoral researcher in O鈥橰eilly鈥檚 lab, the scientists show that the connections in the brain between the prefrontal cortex and the basal ganglia could play a similar role to the pointers used in computer science. The researchers added new information about how the connections between those two regions of the brain could work into their model.
The result was that the model could be trained to understand simple sentences using a select group of words. After the training period, the researchers fed the model new sentences using familiar words in novel ways and found that the model could still comprehend the sentence structure.
While the results show that a pointer-like system could be at play in the brain, the function is not identical to the system used in computer science, the scientists said. It鈥檚 similar to comparing an airplane鈥檚 wing and a bird鈥檚 wing, O鈥橰eilly said. They鈥檙e both used for flying but they work differently.
In the brain, for example, the pointer-like system must still be learned. The brain has to be trained, in this case, to understand sentences while a computer can be programmed to understand sentences immediately.
鈥淎s your brain learns, it gets better and better at processing these novel kinds of information,鈥 O鈥橰eilly said.
Other study co-authors include David Noelle of the University of California, Merced, and Jonathan Cohen of Princeton University. The research was supported by an Intelligence Advanced Research Projects Activity grant through the U.S. Department of the Interior.
Contact:
Randall O鈥橰eilly, 303-492-0054
Randy.Oreilly@colorado.edu
Trent Kriete, 561-302-1737
Trenton.Kriete@colorado.edu
Laura Snider, CU media relations, 303-735-0528
Laura.Snider@colorado.edu